In the United States on Februaury 2nd every year,
if a groundhog sees its shadow then there will be 30 more days of winter, if
not then spring follows immediately. Folklore in relation to atmospheric
conditions has been common place for centuries and in times before accurate
weather prediction could be made, people had a need to find ways of figuring
out what was going to happen to their crop or what the conditions at sea would
be like. Although most of those early ideas and myths are just fairy tales,
like the groundhog story, many are steeped in truth as they were based on
multiple observations over time. For example, “ no weather is ill, if the wind
be still,” when there is a high pressure system, there is cooling air moving
towards the earth’s surface, cool subsiding air is then warmed which may cause
evaporation of clouds and often results fair weather.
Similarly, there have been stories over the centuries of
animals being able to “predict” conditions such as violent storms and earth
quakes. Dogs howling incessantly, caged birds becoming restless, the sound of
insects disappearing as they had all left to find safety elsewhere. These are
anecdotal stories but scientists are starting to research the validity of such
claims.
Here at Oceans Research it was observed on our regular
chumming trips (where our interns get the opportunity to come face to face with
white sharks as they help us collect essential data on population dynamics of
this apex predator), that on the days after heavy storms, there seemed to be
less sharks in the bay. And here at Oceans Research we are all about the
research and not the anecdotes. With a
little searching we came across a paper that was published about juvenile
blacktip sharks, who left their sheltered nursery grounds for deeper water
before the tropical storm Gabriel made landfall, and all the sharks returned
after the storm was over. It was concluded after looking at all possible
environmental variables that the sharks were responding (perhaps innately) to a
drop in atmospheric pressure. The only other paper published in relation to
sharks and pressure showed that vestibular hair cells in dogfish are
responsible for detecting changes in pressure and that these sharks can detect
pressure changes as low as 5 millibars.
We have extensive data sets on the movements of white sharks
in Mossel Bay, with more data coming in everyday using more and more
sophisticated methods for tracking sharks such as acoustic telemetry, where
sharks are tagged and are detected when moving past receivers in the bay (part of the Ocean Tracking Network- OTN), or by
satellite tracking devices (part of the Ocearch expedition). The goal of this part of the project is to compare the movements of the white
sharks in relation to changing atmospheric pressure systems. This is the meat
of my MSc project and hopefully the results will yield some exiting information
about these majestic creatures.
Another observation was made by an aquarist who worked at
the Shark Lab and aquarium of Mossel Bay (more info can be found on this blog
about the Shark Lab and its workings). He noticed that during storms the three
species of benthic (or bottom dwelling) sharks that are on display seemed to
group together more than usual when there was a storm outside. So sticking with
the shark/pressure topic we decided to test this.
When it comes to scientific experimental design there can be
a lot of ups and downs. What was supposed to be a simple experiment turned out
to be one of the most difficult of the Master projects, with the irony being when you
finally get it right you end up moving back to the simple instead of the
complicated, after having discarded all the other parameters.
When looking at the grouping behaviour of sharks in a public
aquarium, you have to take into account the aesthetics of whatever you are
doing. You have a small sample size of sharks to work with at any time, the
roof is low and the tank is wide, so photographic or video footage has to be
taken with a wide angle lens. Can we correct for the fish eyes effect of that
lens?
Figure 1: Using software we can remove the fisheye effect
and measure accurate distances between sharks.
How often are you going to record behaviours. We started at
three times a week, for three hours spread over a day. But what about behaviour
at night? It is presumed that some of these species are nocturnal. Do we really need behaviour metrics such as
swimming patterns when these sharks spend most of their time resting in one
place? Do we somehow divide the tank into quadrants and see which are more
favourable?
Figure 2: An early experimental design
There is a skylight and the sharks tend to be on the
opposite end of the tank to the skylight. Are they moving away from the light
or is it because the tanks water inflow pipe happens to be on the same side as
the skylight? Do we take readings on all days to encompass a more “natural”
behaviour or do we avoid the three days a week that the sharks are fed and
become most active? Do we need to identify individual sharks by using some sort
of marking method or do we look at what they are doing as a collective bunch?
The three benthic species we have in Mossel Bay (in captivity and on the reefs)
tend to group and lie on top of each other as if they didn’t know they are
separate species. With each answer comes a new idea or question and it
compounds so much in trying to design the perfect experiment where all
variables needed are accounted for and those that are not are discarded.
Figure 3: Redesigning
In the end Occam’s Razor often proves to be the way to
go. It basically states that among
competing hypotheses, the one that makes the fewest assumptions should be
selected. And when designing an experiment, a similar idea can come through,
where after removing all that is unnecessary in what is needed in finding the
answer to the question, we are left with a simple experiment that has all the
scientific power necessary to get the research question answered. It also helps
when going through the literature that you find an experiment that someone else
has done which answers even more of the methodological questions you were
struggling with and helps make things even simpler.
So in the end we are looking at the distances between the
sharks at half hour intervals over a 24 hour period once a week to ascertain
how they are grouping, and then comparing that grouping with how much they
congregate during low pressure systems. We are currently in the control phase
and are eagerly awaiting the stormy season to see what results we get.
Figure 4: Almost there
It is also important
to realize for young budding shark scientists and prospective interns which no
doubt will read this blog when looking into coming to Oceans Research to learn
about sharks, that their dream job isn’t just about being on a boat all day and
playing with sharks. Research can be tedious and monotonous with many hours
spent off the water problem solving and conducting experiments which seem
extremely boring, but it all adds up to the great database of life, where
scientists publish their findings and contribute to the global knowledge and
ultimately the protection of the planet and the organisms which inhabit it.
Tristan Scott
MSc candidate. University of Pretoria/Oceans Research
No comments:
Post a Comment